Why do animals that live in caves become blind? This question has long intrigued scientists and been the subject of hot debate. Clearly, across the animal kingdom, blindness has evolved repeatedly. There are thousands of underground and cave-dwelling species, from naked mole rats to bats, found throughout nature. Many of these species have lost their sense of sight.
|
A well-studied blind cavefish (bottom), the Mexican tetra (Astyanax mexicanus), is a small, docile, pink-hued fish just a few centimeters long that could easily make its home in an aquarium. ASU evolutionary biologist Reed Cartwright chose this Mexican tetra because there is also a surface-dwelling form (top) that has retained its sight [Credit: Arizona State University] |
Charles Darwin originally suggested that eyes could be lost by "disuse" over time. Now, Reed Cartwright, an ASU evolutionary biologist in the School of Life Sciences and researcher at the Biodesign Institute, wants to get to the heart of the matter -- and in a recent publication in the journal
BMC Evolutionary Biology, may be proving Darwin wrong.
"We think that blindness in cavefish is indeed Darwinian, but ultimately this disproves Darwin's original hypothesis of 'disuse'," said Cartwright. In new research, Cartwright explains that eyes are not lost by disuse, but rather, demonstrate Darwin's fundamental theory of natural selection at work -- with blindness selected as favorable and the fittest -- for living in a cave.
Go FishFor their work, his research team choose to model a well-studied blind cavefish, the Mexican tetra (Astyanax mexicanus), a small, docile, pink-hued fish just a few centimeters long that could easily make its home in an aquarium.
It's inhabited caves for 2-3 million years, giving it 5 million generations worth of time to evolve blindness. Cartwright's group chose this Mexican tetra because there is also a surface-dwelling form that has retained its sight. And for scientists, this built-in comparative power makes it a good choice for further exploration. They have two populations to study that can interbreed and are polar opposites for physical traits.
So Cartwright's group decided to use computational power to investigate how multiple evolutionary mechanisms interact to shape the fish that live in caves.
"The problem we have in these caves is that they are connected to the surface, and fish that can see immigrate into the cave and bring genes for sight with them," said Cartwright. "Under these conditions, we don't typically expect to find such a difference in traits between surface and cave populations. Unless selection was really, really, strong."
How strong? In their model, the selection for blindness would need to be about 48 times stronger than the immigration rate for Mexican tetras to evolve blindness in caves. Cartwright's group estimates that a measure of fitness for blindness, called the selection coefficient, in the tetra is between 0.5 percent and 50 percent.
These coefficients are high enough that laboratory experiments should have detected a difference between surface and cave forms of the fish; however, none have to date.
Blinded by the lightCartwright's team turned to a hypothesis going all the way back to a letter to the editor of Nature in 1925 by E. Ray Lankester, that essentially stated that the reason you have blindness in caves is because the fish that can see simply leave.
"If sighted fish swim towards the light, the only fish that stay in the cave are blind fish. They aren't trying to get to the light anymore because they can't see it. Which actually is a form of selection, and thus, Darwinian evolution in action," said Cartwright.
According to Cartwright, explaining a fitness difference as big as 10 percent between sighted and blind fish may be difficult, "Iosing eyes might not give you 10 percent more offspring. However, if 10 percent of your seeing eye fish leave the cave, the migration rate is reasonably low, and that could be enough."
If over time, enough of the seeing eye fish are systematically being removed, they will also be removed from the gene pool, and that could be enough to drive the evolutionary process.
It could be this sort of habitat preference that maintains the local blind fish population and the fish that can see are preferentially moving out of the cave. "We found that even a low level of preferential emigration, e.g. two percent, would provide a significant boost to local adaptation and the evolution of blindness in caves."
Cartwright's team hopes that field biologists begin to consider Lankester's 90-year old hypothesis when studying cavefish. "It would be great if someone could develop a study to test Lankester's hypothesis and whether it is driving the evolution of blindness in caves. That would really help answer one of the questions that have intrigued biologists for over a century."
Source: Arizona State University [April 17, 2017]
0 komentar:
Post a Comment